The Toronto Hospital Alertness Test scale: relationship to daytime sleepiness, fatigue, and symptoms of depression and anxiety.
Journal: 2016/February - Nature and Science of Sleep
ISSN: 1179-1608
Abstract:
OBJECTIVE
The Toronto Hospital Alertness Test (THAT) scale was designed to measure alertness, defined as the capacity of the mind to respond appropriately to external and internal stimuli. The present study's aim is to determine normative values of alertness on the THAT and to explore the relationship among excessive daytime sleepiness, fatigue, depressive symptoms, and alertness.
METHODS
Normative data were collected from 60 healthy males and females. To explore the relationship among alertness, daytime sleepiness, fatigue, depression, and anxiety, data were collected from charts of sleep clinic patients. All study subjects completed measures for fatigue, sleepiness, depressive symptoms, and anxiety.
RESULTS
The average score on the THAT was 34.9±7.2 (range 22-50) for the control group. The cutoff score for the THAT, indicative of clinically significant reduced alertness, was determined to be ≤20.5 (mean -2 SD). THAT alertness scores were found to be modestly, significantly, and negatively correlated with fatigue levels (r=-0.39, P<0.001), depressive symptoms (r=-0.53, P<0.001), and anxiety symptoms (r=-0.41, P<0.001). No correlations were found between alertness levels and daytime sleepiness. Regression analyses revealed a significant model (F=19.9, P<0.001, adjusted R (2)=0.35) with depressive symptoms (P<0.001) and fatigue (P=0.006) emerging as the only significant predictors of scores on the THAT.
CONCLUSIONS
The findings of this study support that sleepiness is not the same as poor alertness. Depressive symptoms and fatigue, but not sleepiness, were found to have a strong and significant impact on levels of alertness. This is the first study to link poor alertness to depressive symptoms.
Relations:
Content
Citations
(1)
References
(28)
Affiliates
(2)
Similar articles
Articles by the same authors
Discussion board
Nature and Science of Sleep. Dec/31/2015; 8: 41-45
Published online Jan/18/2016

The Toronto Hospital Alertness Test scale: relationship to daytime sleepiness, fatigue, and symptoms of depression and anxiety

Abstract

Objective

The Toronto Hospital Alertness Test (THAT) scale was designed to measure alertness, defined as the capacity of the mind to respond appropriately to external and internal stimuli. The present study’s aim is to determine normative values of alertness on the THAT and to explore the relationship among excessive daytime sleepiness, fatigue, depressive symptoms, and alertness.

Methods

Normative data were collected from 60 healthy males and females. To explore the relationship among alertness, daytime sleepiness, fatigue, depression, and anxiety, data were collected from charts of sleep clinic patients. All study subjects completed measures for fatigue, sleepiness, depressive symptoms, and anxiety.

Results

The average score on the THAT was 34.9±7.2 (range 22–50) for the control group. The cutoff score for the THAT, indicative of clinically significant reduced alertness, was determined to be ≤20.5 (mean −2 SD). THAT alertness scores were found to be modestly, significantly, and negatively correlated with fatigue levels (r=−0.39, P<0.001), depressive symptoms (r=−0.53, P<0.001), and anxiety symptoms (r=−0.41, P<0.001). No correlations were found between alertness levels and daytime sleepiness. Regression analyses revealed a significant model (F=19.9, P<0.001, adjusted R2=0.35) with depressive symptoms (P<0.001) and fatigue (P=0.006) emerging as the only significant predictors of scores on the THAT.

Conclusion

The findings of this study support that sleepiness is not the same as poor alertness. Depressive symptoms and fatigue, but not sleepiness, were found to have a strong and significant impact on levels of alertness. This is the first study to link poor alertness to depressive symptoms.

Introduction

Alertness is defined as the capacity of the mind to respond appropriately to external and internal stimuli.1 Alertness encompasses facets of attention, vigilance, and cognitive performance; individuals who are alert are better able to perceive and react appropriately to varying situations. In essence, excessive sleepiness is defined as a pathologically increased propensity to fall asleep; the operational definition of excessive sleepiness that was provided in a large epidemiological study includes concepts of ease of falling asleep, periods of onset of uncontrollable sleep, falling asleep during what would normally be a period of wakefulness and an increased need to take naps.2 Our previous work has proposed that daytime sleepiness and alertness are not opposite states of the same phenomena, but are distinct manifestations of different neural pathways and neurochemical mechanisms.1

A recently developed measure of alertness, the Toronto Hospital Alertness Test (THAT) scale, has been shown to be valid and have high re-test reliability in the determination of trait alertness levels.1 This latter study also provides information on the development of the THAT. In a small study3 of 12 healthy volunteers (aged 39±7 years), a mean score of 38±7 was obtained using the THAT. A larger study of 1,010 normal subjects who were free from obstructive sleep apnea, depression, narcolepsy, multiple sclerosis, shift work, or daytime sleepiness found a mean THAT score of 36.9±8.2.4 The findings of this latter larger study is promising, but the study did not explore any relationship the THAT scale may have to depression and fatigue. Also, this latter study was also not specifically designed to define a normal range and cutoff for the THAT. As such, there is yet no normal range and cutoff for the THAT has been defined; a reliable normal range for the THAT will require better designed studies.

Our previous study has suggested that no correlation exists between the THAT and polysomnographic (PSG) measure of sleepiness (as measured using the Multiple Wakefulness Test, MWT).1 The MWT is a measure best suited to assess one’s ability to stay awake. The Maintenance of Wakefulness Test is a PSG measurement of the ability to resist any sleep propensity under soporific conditions and is likely to be yet another measure of the collection of states (eg, sleepiness, alertness, and fatigue). However, Bonnet and Arand5 have proposed that the Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT) is a better measure of sleepiness. The MSLT is a measure of sleep propensity, whereas the MWT is a measure of ability to remain awake.6

We have previously shown1 a modest but significant correlation between alertness (as measured using the THAT) and symptoms of depression (as evaluated using the Centre for Epidemiological Studies in Depression [CES-D] screening instrument7). This finding has resulted in the hypothesis that mood may be a key influence on alertness. Symptoms of anxiety were not examined in the aforementioned study. As such, the aforementioned hypothesis warrants exploration using measures of both anxiety and depressive symptoms.

A further key quandary is the question of what association, if any, exists between cognitive fatigue and alertness. Fatigue is a ubiquitous complaint in mood disorders8,9 and in those with sleep pathology.10 Fatigue levels have been observed to significantly impact the ability to maintain sustained attention in several patient populations.11,12 Yet, the link between fatigue and cognitive performance remains uncertain; some studies found no relationship13,14 while others report a negative association.15,16

The aim of the present study is to further explore the relationship among alertness, sleepiness mood, and fatigue.

Methods

Ethics approval for this study was obtained from the University Health Network Research Ethics Board. As this was a retrospective clinical chart review study no informed consent from patients was required as there was no direct contact with patients. To obtain normative values for the THAT, data were collected from 60 healthy males and females ranging in age from 17 to 74 years. To explore the relationship among alertness, sleepiness, fatigue, and symptoms consistent with depression and anxiety, data were also collected from patients who were referred to our clinic for investigation of a sleep disorder and who subsequently underwent overnight PSG testing. Clinical charts were consecutively selected for inclusion in the study – a total of 458 charts were reviewed, but 194 were excluded due to missing data on one or more of the major outcome variables. Patients’ charts contained responses to a battery of self-report scales that measured: alertness – THAT;1 depressive symptoms – CES-D;7 anxiety symptoms – Zung Self-Rating Anxiety Scale (SAS);17 daytime sleepiness – Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS);18 fatigue – Fatigue Severity Scale (FSS).19

Results from daytime PSG sleep latency testing (as measured using the MSLT)20 were available for 210 patients. The questionnaires in the charts had been completed on the day of the consultation with the sleep specialist and the PSG testing took place 2–4 weeks later.

The following cutoffs were used for this study: Scores ≥16 is indicative of depression based on the CES-D;7 ≤44 represents the normal range of the SAS;17 ≥8 signifies clinical sleepiness for a sleep clinic population based on the ESS;21 and, for the FSS, the normal average healthy adult score is 2.3±0.7,19 with a cutoff >3.7 (representing the mean +2 SD) indicative of pathological fatigue.

Statistical analyses

Statistical testing was conducted using the SPSS statistical program (version 19, IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). Differences in means were obtained using the Student’s t-test. Relationships between variables were explored using Pearson’s Correlation and Regression Analyses were performed to determine the degree to which mood (symptoms of depression based on the CES-D and anxiety based on the SAS), daytime sleepiness, fatigue, age, and sex contribute to levels of alertness. The level of statistical significance was set at a more stringent level of P<0.01 to avoid Type 1 error due to multiple statistical testing of the dataset. Effect size was determined using Cohen’s d22 which provides the standardized mean effect and expresses the mean difference between two groups in SD units: effect sizes between 0.5 and 0.8 are moderate and above 0.8 are considered large.22

Results

Normative values: control subjects

Possible scores on the THAT range from 0 to 50. Normative values of the THAT were collected from 30 males and 30 females ranging in age from 17 to 74 years (average 39.1±14.3 years). The average score on the THAT was 34.9±7.2 (range 22–50). Based on the formula of two times the SD subtracted from the mean, the cutoff of the THAT, indicative of clinically significant reduced alertness, is 20.5. No sex differences were noted and scores on the THAT did not differ with age, with no significant differences in the THAT score between those below and above 40 years of age.

Relationship to sleepiness, fatigue, depression, and anxiety: sleep clinic patients

Of the 264 patient charts included in this study, 54% (n=142) of the patients were women and 46% (n=122) were men, and the average age of the group was 45.3±14.2 years. The results from the questionnaires and the average daytime sleep onset latency from the four MSLT sessions are shown in Table 1.

The group was divided into those with normal and impaired alertness based on the cutoff of 20.5 for the THAT (Table 2). A significant difference between those with normal and impaired alertness was found for all variables except for the ESS and the MSLT sleep onset latency. Effect size calculation (using Cohen’s d) (Table 2) indicates large differences in fatigue (FSS) and depressive symptoms (CES-D scale) and moderate differences in anxiety (SAS) between those with normal and those with impaired alertness.

Correlational analyses (Table 3) revealed that alertness (as measured using the THAT scale) was modestly, significantly, and negatively correlated with fatigue levels (FSS), depressive symptoms (CES-D), and anxiety symptoms (SAS). A strong, significant, and positive correlation was also noted between scores on the CES-D and the SAS, and the FSS was modestly but positively and significantly correlated with scores on the CES-D and the SAS.

To further explore the relationship among alertness, fatigue, sleepiness, depressive symptoms, and anxiety complaints, regression analyses were performed. A significant model was evident (F=19.9, P<0.001, adjusted R2=0.35) with scores on the CES-D (P<0.001) and FSS (P=0.006) scales emerging as significant predictors of scores on the THAT scale. Neither ESS (P=0.98) nor FSS (P=0.29) were found to be significant predictor variables. The model incorporating the ESS, FSS, CES-D, and SAS was found to explain half (50%, adjusted R2=0.50) of the variability in scores on the THAT. Conversely, a model incorporating age, sex, Apnea–Hypopnea Index, and MSLT sleep latency was found to explain only 16% (adjusted R2=0.16) of the variability in scores on the THAT.

Discussion

Daytime sleepiness and poor alertness are used interchangeably in the literature.23 However, the findings of this study support our previous hypothesis that daytime sleepiness is not the same as poor alertness. Daytime sleepiness, whether measured subjectively with the ESS or physiologically from the sleep onset latency obtained using the MSLT, did not differ among those with and without impaired alertness and did not predict scores on the THAT, thereby confirming that alertness is a separate construct from and not merely the opposite of sleepiness.

This study is the first to quantitatively define normal levels of alertness. Alertness is important for appropriate on-task performance and for the prevention of accidents. Unfortunately, there is as yet agreed-upon definition of alertness and no gold standard for the measurement of alertness. As such, the definition of the construct of alertness is still controversial and further research in this area is needed to clarify exactly how alertness differs from sleepiness. The purpose of this paper has been to show what steps we have taken to further characterize the concept of alertness as an entity distinct from sleepiness.

There is strong evidence that performance is not solely dependent on the degree of sleepiness. Pejovic et al24 investigated the impact of recovery sleep of 10 hours per night for three nights following six nights of mild sleep restriction (6 hours/night) in healthy young adults. Subjects did not exhibit any objective or subjective sleepiness after the recovery sleep, but their performance on a psychomotor vigilance task remained significantly impaired. This study demonstrates that with sleep restriction, recovery of neurocognitive deficits such as impaired performance may be slower when compared to improvements in excessive sleepiness. The continued performance decrements in the absence of sleepiness emphasizes the lack of association between sleepiness and vigilance and points to the strong influence of factors other than daytime sleepiness on performance. Alertness is potentially a good candidate factor, but further study will be needed to validate this hypothesis. Certainly, the aforementioned study emphasizes the need to look beyond sleepiness as the primary determinant of performance and an exploration of other factors such as alertness to characterize how sleep can impact performance is warranted.

In contrast to sleepiness, symptoms of depression and fatigue among our study subjects were found to have a strong and significant impact on levels of alertness. An association between fatigue and symptoms consistent with depression was noted. This may be expected as fatigue is a major symptom of depressive disorders.25 However, there is as yet no information on poor alertness as a symptom of depressive disorders.

Depressive disorders are characterized by impairments in both vigilance26 and attention.27 A study from our group reported that patients with untreated major depressive disorder performed poorly on the driving simulator.28 Greater workplace injuries and accidents have also been noted among firefighters with more depressive symptoms.29 The above could be explained by a reduction in alertness in those with depressive disorder as alertness has been linked to reduced visuospacial attention.30 Along these lines, our study31 has shown that treatment with mirtazapine improves driving performance despite the increased short-term sedation associated with mirtazapine.

Conclusion

The current study underscores that sleepiness and alertness are separate phenomena and not opposite poles of the same continuum. One limitation of this study is the use of only questionnaire data to explore alertness in sleep clinic patients. A further limitation is the use of only 60 control subjects to determine normative values of the THAT. It is notable that despite this low number of subjects, the cutoff of 20.5 for the THAT (calculated as twice the SD subtracted from the mean) obtained in our study is identical to the cutoff of 20.5 for the THAT calculated based on a larger study of 1,010 normal subjects.4 With an established normative values for alertness, further studies can utilize these to investigate linkages between impaired alertness and changes in sleep architectural variables in patients with sleep disorders, particularly in insomnia patients where fatigue, but not daytime sleepiness, predominates. Additionally, given that symptoms indicative of depression are predictive of levels of alertness, future studies are also needed to explore the contribution of impaired alertness to neurovegetative symptoms in individuals with depressed mood.

Table 1
Questionnaire and MSLT results
MeasureAverage (± SD)% above cutoff
THAT – alertness24.5±9.236.7 (THAT ≤20.5)
ESS – EDS9.4±5.741.1 (ESS >10)
MSLT – EDS10.7±5.373.7 (SOL ≤10 min)
FSS – fatigue5.0±1.383.5 (FSS ≥3.7)
CES-D – depressive symptoms21.6±13.459.9 (CES-D ≥16)
SAS – anxiety symptoms35.9±13.024.0 (SAS ≥45)

Abbreviations: THAT, Toronto Hospital Alertness Test; ESS, Epworth Sleepiness Scale; MSLT, Multiple Sleep Latency Test; SOL, sleep onset latency; FSS, Fatigue Severity Scale; CES-D, Centre for Epidemiological Studies in Depression; SAS, Zung Self-Rating Anxiety Scale; EDS, excessive daytime sleepiness.

Table 2
Normal and impaired alertness across study variables
MeasureNormal alertness
Impaired alertness
Significance (P-value)Effect size (Cohen’s d)
Average (± SD)Average(± SD)
ESS9.3±5.49.6±6.20.7−0.052
MSLT10.2±5.511.4±4.90.1−0.230
FSS4.6±1.35.7±1.0<0.001−0.948
CES-D16.4±10.930.6±12.5<0.001−1.211
SAS32.5±12.341.9±12.0<0.001−0.774

Abbreviations: ESS, Epworth Sleepiness Scale; MSLT, Multiple Sleep Latency Test; FSS, Fatigue Severity Scale; CES-D, Centre for Epidemiological Studies in Depression; SAS, Zung Self-Rating Anxiety Scale.

Table 3
Correlational analyses
MeasureCES-DSASESSFSSMSLT
THAT−0.5−0.4−0.37−0.40.03
P<0.001P<0.001P=0.7P<0.001P=0.8
CES-D0.610.060.3−0.07
P<0.001P=0.6P=0.005P=0.6
SAS0.610.030.3−0.03
P<0.001P=0.7P=0.02P=0.8
ESS0.060.030.2−0.01
P=0.6P=0.7P=0.09P=0.1
FSS0.30.30.2−0.05
P=0.005P=0.02P=0.09P=0.7
MSLT−0.07−0.03−0.01−0.05
P=0.6P=0.8P=0.1P=0.7
AHI0.20.20.10.1−0.03
P=0.03P=0.03P=0.2P=0.2P=0.8

Abbreviations: THAT, Toronto Hospital Alertness Test; ESS, Epworth Sleepiness Scale; MSLT, Multiple Sleep Latency Test; FSS, Fatigue Severity Scale; CES-D, Centre for Epidemiological Studies in Depression; SAS, Zung Self-Rating Anxiety Scale; AHI, Apnea–Hypopnea Index.

Footnotes

Disclosure

The authors report no conflicts of interest in this work.

Acknowledgments

The abstract of this paper was presented at the 27th Annual Meeting of the Associated Professional Sleep Societies, Baltimore, MD; 2013. The abstract was published in Journal of Sleep and Sleep Disorders Research, vol 36, 2013.

References

  • 1. ShapiroCMAuchCReimerMA new approach to the construct of alertnessJ Psychosom Res2006606595603[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 2. OhayonMMDauvilliersYReynoldsCF3rdOperational definitions and algorithms for excessive sleepiness in the general population: implications for DSM-5 nosologyArch Gen Psychiat2012697179[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 3. IonescuDDriverHSHeonEFlanaganJShapiroCMSleep and daytime sleepiness in retinitis pigmentosa patientsJ Sleep Res2001104329335[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 4. DeanBAguilarDShapiroCMImpaired health status, daily functioning, and work productivity in adults with excessive sleepinessJ Occup Environ Med2010522144149[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 5. BonnetMHArandDLImpact of motivation on Multiple Sleep Latency Test and Maintenance of Wakefulness Test measurementsJ Clin Sleep Med200514386390[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 6. LittnerMRKushidaCWiseMStandards of Practice Committee of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Practice parameters for clinical use of the Multiple Sleep Latency Test and the maintenance of wakefulness testSleep2005281113121[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 7. RadloffLThe CES-D Scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general populationApp Psychol Meas1977385401[Google Scholar]
  • 8. ValentineADMeyersCACognitive and mood disturbance as causes and symptoms of fatigue in cancer patientsCancer20019216941698[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 9. BardwellWAMoorePAncoli-IsraelSDimsdaleJEFatigue in obstructive sleep apnea: driven by depressive symptoms instead of apnea severity?Am J Psychiat2003160350355[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 10. HossainJLAhmadPReinishLWKayumovLHossainNKShapiroCMSubjective fatigue and subjective sleepiness: two independent consequences of sleep disorders?J Sleep Res2005143245253[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 11. IckmansKMeeusMKosDCognitive performance is of clinical importance, but is unrelated to pain severity in women with chronic fatigue syndromeClin Rheumatol2013321014751485[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 12. SinclairKLPonsfordJLRajaratnamSMAndersonCSustained attention following traumatic brain injury: use of the Psychomotor Vigilance TaskJ Clin Exp Neuropsychol2013352210224[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 13. McDonaldECopeHDavidACognitive impairment in patients with chronic fatigue: a preliminary studyJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiat1993567812815[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 14. ShortKMcCabeMTooleyGCognitive functioning in chronic fatigue syndrome and the role of depression, anxiety, and fatigueJ Psychosom Res200226475483[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 15. JoyceEBlumenthalSWesselySMemory, attention, and executive function in chronic fatigue syndromeJ Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry1996605495503[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 16. CapuronLWelbergLHeimCCognitive dysfunction relates to subjective report of mental fatigue in patients with chronic fatigue syndromeNeuropsychopharmacology200631817771784[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 17. ZungWWKA rating instrument for anxiety disordersPsychosomatics197112371379[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 18. JohnsMWA new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth Sleepiness ScaleSleep1991146540545[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 19. KruppLBLaRoccaNGMuir-NashJSteinbergADThe Fatigue Severity Scale. Application to patients with multiple sclerosis and systemic lupus erythematosusArch Neurol19894611211123[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 20. RichardsonGSCarskadonMAFlaggWVan den HoedJDementWCMitlerMMExcessive daytime sleepiness in man: multiple sleep latency measurement in narcoleptic and control subjectsElectroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol1978455621627[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 21. RosenthalLDDolanDCThe Epworth Sleepiness Scale in the identification of obstructive sleep apneaJ Nerv Ment Dis20081965429431[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 22. CohenJStatistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences2nd edHillsdale, NJLawrence Earlbaum Associates1998
  • 23. Aston-JonesGBrain structures and receptors involved in alertnessSleep Med20056Suppl 1S3S7[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 24. PejovicSBastaMVgontzasANEffects of recovery sleep after one work week of mild sleep restriction on interleukin-6 and cortisol secretion and daytime sleepiness and performanceAm J Physiol Endocrinol Metab20133057E890E896[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 25. American Psychiatric AssociationDiagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders4th edWashington, DC2000
  • 26. KertzmanSReznikIHornik-LurieTWeizmanAKotlerMAmitalDStroop performance in major depression: selective attention impairment or psychomotor slowness?J Affect Disord20101221–2167173[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 27. GodardJGrondinSBaruchPLafleurMFPsychosocial and neurocognitive profiles in depressed patients with major depressive disorder and bipolar disorderPsychiatry Res20111902–3244252[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 28. BulmashELMollerHJKayumovLShenJWangXShapiroCMPsychomotor disturbance in depression: assessment using a driving simulator paradigmJ Affect Disord2006931–3213218[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 29. ChungYKParkCYThe effects of injury and accidents on self-rated depression in male municipal firefightersSaf Health Work201122158168[PubMed][Google Scholar]
  • 30. SchockLSchwenzerMSturmWMathiakKAlertness and visuospatial attention in clinical depressionBMC Psychiat20111178[Google Scholar]
  • 31. ShenJMollerHJWangXMirtazapine, a sedating antidepressant, and improved driving safety in patients with major depressive disorder: a prospective, randomized trial of 28 patientsJ Clin Psychiatry2009703370377[PubMed][Google Scholar]
Collaboration tool especially designed for Life Science professionals.Drag-and-drop any entity to your messages.